Dahlstrom, W. G. (1993). Tests: Small Samples, Large Consequences. American Psychologist, 4, 393-399.
Human decision making is a complex process, and one which often has large consequences, both positive and negative. However, this process is not uncommonly fraught with errors of judgment. Psychological tests can serve as a means to offset the impact of these distortions. The article gives several examples of proper and improper use of tests, such as the use of the Stanford-Binet to determine a defendant was not fit to stand trial as an adult, overturning recommendations formerly made by a psychiatrist employing informal ad hoc tests. They define a proper psychological test as meeting six key criteria: using standardized materials and procedures, ensuring optimal motivation in test-takers, recording data immediately, scoring objectively, and establishing test norms. The article is summarized nicely: "The samples of behavior that psychologists collect in the brief time that an hourglass takes to empty have been shown to reveal basic aspects of ability, personality, and temperament that are operative over long spans of an individual's life. Proper gathering of these data by means of well-executed administrations of standardized test instruments can provide gatekeepers with invaluable information to minimize risks of errors of judgment in decisions about their clients and increase the range of predictions that can have large consequences in the lives of those with whom they deal."
Friday, October 16, 2009
Sunday, September 27, 2009
Psychiatric Resident Conceptualizations of Mood and Affect within the Mental Status Examination
Serby, M. (2003). Psychiatric Resident Conceptualizations of Mood and Affect within the Mental Status Examination. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1527-1529.
In the Mental Status Exam (MSE), affect is conceptualized as "external, objective, visible emotional tone. It is also the moment-to-moment measure, while may be labile or constricted, congruent or not with expressed ideas, and may be varied during any interview." Mood, on the other hand, is conceptualized as "an internal, subjective, and sustained emotional state and should be reported as such". However, this study revealed that psychiatric residents do not typically abide strictly to these definitions. They tend to understand that mood is subjective/internal and affect is objective/external, but appear to be less focused on the temporal distinction between the two.
In the Mental Status Exam (MSE), affect is conceptualized as "external, objective, visible emotional tone. It is also the moment-to-moment measure, while may be labile or constricted, congruent or not with expressed ideas, and may be varied during any interview." Mood, on the other hand, is conceptualized as "an internal, subjective, and sustained emotional state and should be reported as such". However, this study revealed that psychiatric residents do not typically abide strictly to these definitions. They tend to understand that mood is subjective/internal and affect is objective/external, but appear to be less focused on the temporal distinction between the two.
Labels:
4 Stars,
Assessment,
Clinical Psychology,
Doug,
Emotion,
Mood
Evidence-Based Assessment of Pediatric Bipolar Disorder
Youngstrom, E.A. & Duax, J. (2005). Evidence-Based Assessment of Pediatric Bipolar Disorder, Part I: Base Rate and Family History. Journal of American Academic Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 712-716.
Bipolar disorder is rare in children before puberty, there is controversy about how to diagnose it, and there are few published clinical trials to guide treatment. Additionally, there is evidence that use of stimulants or antidepressants might worsen the course of illness; the compounds most likely to be effective also have the potential for serious side effects and therefore should not be prescribed unless one is confident in the diagnosis and the potential for benefit. On the flip side, there are strong concerns that untreated bipolar disorder will follow a progressive and deteorating course. Therefore, diagnosis of pediatric bipolar disorder (PBD) is both controversial and high-stakes.
This article discusses how one might implement recommendations of Evidenced-Based Practice (EBP) to gather additional information and integrate it in order to obtain greater confidence regarding further testing or treatment. A recent meta-analysis indicates that children with a first-degree relative with bipolar have a 5-fold increase in risk, and children with a second-degree relative with bipolar have a 2.5-fold risk increase. Another recent meta-analysis reveals that no other risk factors besides family history have been sufficiently documented to justify integration into clinical decision-making. Therefore, combining the child's familial information with current diagnostic base rates (which can be obtained from publications reporting rates from similar demographies, clinical settings, and interviewing techniques), and using Bayesian methods can yield probabilities that the specific individual has the diagnosis (or alternately, the frequency with which people showing that test result at that particular clinic would have the condition). If the probability yielded is below the test/no-test threshold, then no further testing may be required. If the number is above the treatment threshold, then treatment may be warranted. (Note: Often the treatment threshold is determined by the clinician in consultation with the family, weighing information about the costs and benefits of treatment.) If the number falls in between these thresholds, then further assessment may be prudent.
Bipolar disorder is rare in children before puberty, there is controversy about how to diagnose it, and there are few published clinical trials to guide treatment. Additionally, there is evidence that use of stimulants or antidepressants might worsen the course of illness; the compounds most likely to be effective also have the potential for serious side effects and therefore should not be prescribed unless one is confident in the diagnosis and the potential for benefit. On the flip side, there are strong concerns that untreated bipolar disorder will follow a progressive and deteorating course. Therefore, diagnosis of pediatric bipolar disorder (PBD) is both controversial and high-stakes.
This article discusses how one might implement recommendations of Evidenced-Based Practice (EBP) to gather additional information and integrate it in order to obtain greater confidence regarding further testing or treatment. A recent meta-analysis indicates that children with a first-degree relative with bipolar have a 5-fold increase in risk, and children with a second-degree relative with bipolar have a 2.5-fold risk increase. Another recent meta-analysis reveals that no other risk factors besides family history have been sufficiently documented to justify integration into clinical decision-making. Therefore, combining the child's familial information with current diagnostic base rates (which can be obtained from publications reporting rates from similar demographies, clinical settings, and interviewing techniques), and using Bayesian methods can yield probabilities that the specific individual has the diagnosis (or alternately, the frequency with which people showing that test result at that particular clinic would have the condition). If the probability yielded is below the test/no-test threshold, then no further testing may be required. If the number is above the treatment threshold, then treatment may be warranted. (Note: Often the treatment threshold is determined by the clinician in consultation with the family, weighing information about the costs and benefits of treatment.) If the number falls in between these thresholds, then further assessment may be prudent.
Friday, May 1, 2009
The effect of exercise on depression, anxiety, and other mood states
Byrne, A. & Byrne, D.G. (1993). The effect of exercise on depression, anxiety, and other mood states. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37, 565-574.
This review supports the claim that exercise treatments are associated with positive psychological benefits for both clinical and non-clinical populations. Although most of the studies employed aerobic interventions, some studies even showed positive improvements associated with non-aerobic exercise (e.g. weight lifting). However, all of these results need to be interpreted with caution as the result of methodological limitations and indirect evidence. If future studies were to show unequivocally positive psychological gains are caused by exercise interventions, including exercise for people with affective disorders will undoubtedly have a number of advantages since it is time and cost effective by comparison to psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy, comes with few side effects if done correctly, and may even by used to prophylactically prevent the occurrence of future affective episodes.
This review supports the claim that exercise treatments are associated with positive psychological benefits for both clinical and non-clinical populations. Although most of the studies employed aerobic interventions, some studies even showed positive improvements associated with non-aerobic exercise (e.g. weight lifting). However, all of these results need to be interpreted with caution as the result of methodological limitations and indirect evidence. If future studies were to show unequivocally positive psychological gains are caused by exercise interventions, including exercise for people with affective disorders will undoubtedly have a number of advantages since it is time and cost effective by comparison to psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy, comes with few side effects if done correctly, and may even by used to prophylactically prevent the occurrence of future affective episodes.
Labels:
3 Stars,
Affect,
Anxiety,
Clinical Psychology,
Depression,
Doug,
Ecological Psychology,
Emotion,
Stress
Wednesday, April 29, 2009
Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex
Radley, J.J. et al. (2005). Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 313-320.
The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) plays an important role in higher cognitive processes and in the regulation of stress-induced HPA axis activity. This study investigated the effect of stress on dendritic spine density in the mPFC. Rats were restrained for 6 hours daily for 21 days with wire mesh. Following the 21 days of stress, stressed rats weighed less than controls, had a 20% decrease in overall apical dendritic length, a 16% decrease in apical dendritic spine density, and hence an estimated 33% reduction in the total number of axospinous synapses on apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons in the mPFC. These morphological changes may have a significant impact on the functional properties of this region. Clinically, mPFC dysfunction is associated with PTSD and depression. One potential neuroanatomical substrate relevant to these disorders is the mPFC-amygdala circuit. Normally, the mPFC may inhibit amygdala output through its connections on the GABAergic intercalated cells at the border of the lateral and central nuclei of the amygdala. Experimental lesions of the mPFC support this, leading to an enhancement of amygdala-dependent behaviors such as emotionality and fear conditioning. Future studies are needed to investigate the extent to which these morphological changes from chronic stress are reversible.
The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) plays an important role in higher cognitive processes and in the regulation of stress-induced HPA axis activity. This study investigated the effect of stress on dendritic spine density in the mPFC. Rats were restrained for 6 hours daily for 21 days with wire mesh. Following the 21 days of stress, stressed rats weighed less than controls, had a 20% decrease in overall apical dendritic length, a 16% decrease in apical dendritic spine density, and hence an estimated 33% reduction in the total number of axospinous synapses on apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons in the mPFC. These morphological changes may have a significant impact on the functional properties of this region. Clinically, mPFC dysfunction is associated with PTSD and depression. One potential neuroanatomical substrate relevant to these disorders is the mPFC-amygdala circuit. Normally, the mPFC may inhibit amygdala output through its connections on the GABAergic intercalated cells at the border of the lateral and central nuclei of the amygdala. Experimental lesions of the mPFC support this, leading to an enhancement of amygdala-dependent behaviors such as emotionality and fear conditioning. Future studies are needed to investigate the extent to which these morphological changes from chronic stress are reversible.
Comparisons of Stimulus Learning and Response Learning in a Punishment Situation
Bolles, R.C., Holtz, R., Dunn, T., & Hill, W. (1980). Comparisons of Stimulus Learning and Response Learning in a Punishment Situation. Learning and Motivation, 11, 78-96.
Early on in the study of learning, learning was believed to consist of the attachment of a response to a stimuli (an S-R association). Later, in contrast with the long-held conventional view that all learning was of the S-R form, alternative forms were proposed by Pavlov and others. Examples include stimulus learning (S-S*) and response learning (R-S*). What type of learning underlied punishment, for example? This stimulated debate. In common punishment paradigms, it became obvious that is was unclear whether the animal was learning that shock is correlated there contextually with the bar (stimulus) or whether shock is correlated with its behavior of pressing the bar (response). The purpose of this paper was to attempt to disentangle these different forms of learning experimentally. Four novel experimental paradigms were explored.
Experiment 1 contained one single bar in the chamber which could either be pressed or pulled. Animals had to alternate their behavioral response (press to pull, and back again) in order to be rewarded. Punishment was delivered on every tenth press for half the animals or every tenth pull for the other half. Results showed a rapid initial suppression (fear to environmental stimuli), but later a return to baseline for the unpunishment response and continued suppression for the punished response. This seems to be evidence for both types of learning taking place within one paradigm. Experiment 2 simply adjusted the response contingencies (up and down) to see if results would be sensitive to this type of experimental manipulation. Instead of an FR-10 punishment schedule, rats were shocked on FR-4 or FR-25. FR-4 showed dramatic differences in responding from the outset. FR-25 differences only emerged in the second day of punishment.
Experiment 3 used two bars, each of which could be either pressed or pulled. Thus, four punishment conditions were possible, punishing a left-press, a left-lift, a right-press, or a right-lift. Results showed that when a rat was punished for a left-lift, for example, it quickly stopped lifting AND pressing the left bar. However, it continued to lift AND press the right bar. Thus, learning, in this case, seems to be mostly about stimuli. Experiment 4, like Experiment 2, changed the contingencies. Punishment was shifted from an FR-1 schedule to an FR-10 schedule. Punishment conditions were: press or lift left bar, press or lift right bar, lifting left or right, and pressing left or right. Across the conditions, the general trend that emerged was a rapid emergence of stimulus learning and then a slower but undeniable development of response learning.
Early on in the study of learning, learning was believed to consist of the attachment of a response to a stimuli (an S-R association). Later, in contrast with the long-held conventional view that all learning was of the S-R form, alternative forms were proposed by Pavlov and others. Examples include stimulus learning (S-S*) and response learning (R-S*). What type of learning underlied punishment, for example? This stimulated debate. In common punishment paradigms, it became obvious that is was unclear whether the animal was learning that shock is correlated there contextually with the bar (stimulus) or whether shock is correlated with its behavior of pressing the bar (response). The purpose of this paper was to attempt to disentangle these different forms of learning experimentally. Four novel experimental paradigms were explored.
Experiment 1 contained one single bar in the chamber which could either be pressed or pulled. Animals had to alternate their behavioral response (press to pull, and back again) in order to be rewarded. Punishment was delivered on every tenth press for half the animals or every tenth pull for the other half. Results showed a rapid initial suppression (fear to environmental stimuli), but later a return to baseline for the unpunishment response and continued suppression for the punished response. This seems to be evidence for both types of learning taking place within one paradigm. Experiment 2 simply adjusted the response contingencies (up and down) to see if results would be sensitive to this type of experimental manipulation. Instead of an FR-10 punishment schedule, rats were shocked on FR-4 or FR-25. FR-4 showed dramatic differences in responding from the outset. FR-25 differences only emerged in the second day of punishment.
Experiment 3 used two bars, each of which could be either pressed or pulled. Thus, four punishment conditions were possible, punishing a left-press, a left-lift, a right-press, or a right-lift. Results showed that when a rat was punished for a left-lift, for example, it quickly stopped lifting AND pressing the left bar. However, it continued to lift AND press the right bar. Thus, learning, in this case, seems to be mostly about stimuli. Experiment 4, like Experiment 2, changed the contingencies. Punishment was shifted from an FR-1 schedule to an FR-10 schedule. Punishment conditions were: press or lift left bar, press or lift right bar, lifting left or right, and pressing left or right. Across the conditions, the general trend that emerged was a rapid emergence of stimulus learning and then a slower but undeniable development of response learning.
Wednesday, April 1, 2009
Getting comfortable with conversations about race and ethnicity in psychotherapy
Cardemil, E. V., & Battle, C. L. (2003). Guess who’s coming to therapy? Getting comfortable with conversations about race and ethnicity in psychotherapy. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 278-286.
This article urges therapists to engage in open conversations with their clients about race and ethnicity as it applies to the client, the therapist, and the therapeutic alliance. By taking a more active stance and initiating such discussions, especially early in treatment, the therapist may enjoy improved treatment retention, therapeutic alliance, and treatment outcome. After defining race and ethnicity as similar but distinct constructs, the article acknowledges that such conversations will vary in terms of frequency and intensity over different clients and times. It then goes on to provide six recommendations for becoming more comfortable and knowledgeable with having such discussions.
First, it is acknowledged that a client's racial/ethnic background may not be obvious and that it is best to suspend preconceptions about a client and their family members. It is recommended that clients be asked early on in therapy how they identify themselves. Second, it is acknowledged that wide variability exists within racial and ethnic groups and that a client's racial identity development and acculturation process may change over time, thus affecting therapy. Third, it is important to consider how the therapist's own racial/ethnic background may affect the therapeutic process in terms of differences in communication styles and conceptualization of mental health/illness, self, and family/community. Fourth, it is acknowledged that racism, power, and privilege can affect the therapeutic process and that failing to acknowledge such societal issues may invalidate a client's painful personal experiences. Fifth, it is recommended that a client expressing reticence and/or frustration with the topics of race and ethnicity be met with an open and non-defensive explanation that such topics are relevant to many clients, but needn't be pursued if they are found irrelevant or uncomfortable. Lastly, resources for further education/training in race and ethnicity are provided.
This article urges therapists to engage in open conversations with their clients about race and ethnicity as it applies to the client, the therapist, and the therapeutic alliance. By taking a more active stance and initiating such discussions, especially early in treatment, the therapist may enjoy improved treatment retention, therapeutic alliance, and treatment outcome. After defining race and ethnicity as similar but distinct constructs, the article acknowledges that such conversations will vary in terms of frequency and intensity over different clients and times. It then goes on to provide six recommendations for becoming more comfortable and knowledgeable with having such discussions.
First, it is acknowledged that a client's racial/ethnic background may not be obvious and that it is best to suspend preconceptions about a client and their family members. It is recommended that clients be asked early on in therapy how they identify themselves. Second, it is acknowledged that wide variability exists within racial and ethnic groups and that a client's racial identity development and acculturation process may change over time, thus affecting therapy. Third, it is important to consider how the therapist's own racial/ethnic background may affect the therapeutic process in terms of differences in communication styles and conceptualization of mental health/illness, self, and family/community. Fourth, it is acknowledged that racism, power, and privilege can affect the therapeutic process and that failing to acknowledge such societal issues may invalidate a client's painful personal experiences. Fifth, it is recommended that a client expressing reticence and/or frustration with the topics of race and ethnicity be met with an open and non-defensive explanation that such topics are relevant to many clients, but needn't be pursued if they are found irrelevant or uncomfortable. Lastly, resources for further education/training in race and ethnicity are provided.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)